top of page

A Critical Evaluation of Snake Avoidance Training in Dogs: The Case for Positive Reinforcement

Writer's picture: The PATS TeamThe PATS Team

Updated: Jan 24

Dogs play an integral role in the lives of many individuals, serving as companions, working animals, and even sources of emotional support. While training is essential for ensuring that dogs are well-behaved and integrated into human environments, teaching specific behaviours—such as snake avoidance—presents unique challenges. This article critically evaluates traditional snake avoidance training methods, particularly punishment-based approaches, and argues for the application of evidence-based, humane practices rooted in positive reinforcement.

A snake with his head held up high.
Beautiful specimen

The Science of Learning in Dogs

Understanding how dogs learn is fundamental to evaluating different training methodologies. Operant conditioning, as described by B.F. Skinner (1938), outlines four primary quadrants of learning:

  1. Positive Reinforcement (+R): Adding a pleasant stimulus to increase the likelihood of a behaviour.

  2. Negative Reinforcement (-R): Removing an unpleasant stimulus to increase a behaviour.

  3. Positive Punishment (+P): Adding an aversive stimulus to decrease a behaviour.

  4. Negative Punishment (-P): Removing a pleasant stimulus to decrease a behaviour.


Punishment-based snake avoidance training primarily relies on positive punishment (+P), where an aversive stimulus, such as a shock from an e-collar, is applied when a dog approaches or engages with a snake. This approach aims to suppress undesirable behaviours (e.g., investigating or chasing snakes) but fails to consider critical factors, such as the dog's emotional well-being, the ethical implications of aversive training methods, and the long-term efficacy of punishment in behaviour modification.


Problems with Punishment-Based Snake Avoidance Training: Assumptions About Canine Behaviour

Punishment-based approaches operate on the assumption that all dogs will naturally engage with or investigate snakes. This generalisation ignores individual variability in canine behaviour. Furthermore, these methods focus exclusively on suppressing undesirable behaviours without teaching the dog an alternative, desirable response to the presence of a snake.


Fear Conditioning and Generalisation

Punishment can lead to unintended associations due to fear conditioning. Studies on context fear conditioning (e.g., Kim & Fanselow, 1992) have shown that animals subjected to aversive stimuli may generalise their fear to the environment, objects, or individuals present during the punishment.

In the context of snake avoidance training, this means a dog subjected to a shock might:

  • Associate the aversive experience with the location where training occurred, leading to fear of specific terrains or environments.

  • Develop mistrust toward their handler or humans in general, particularly if the handler administers the punishment.

This generalised fear can have long-lasting implications for the dog’s overall well-being and may extend beyond the specific context of snake encounters.


Suppression vs. Elimination of Behaviour

Punishment suppresses behaviour rather than eliminating it. For example, while a shock may temporarily prevent a dog from approaching a snake, it does not address the underlying motivation (e.g., curiosity, prey drive). Once the punishment is removed, the behaviour is likely to return to its previous strength—a phenomenon known as post-punishment over-recovery (Azrin & Holz, 1966).


Moreover, punishment does not modify the reinforcement contingency that previously maintained the behaviour. Without addressing the underlying reinforcement, the behaviour remains intact and will likely re-emerge in the absence of punishment.


Long-Term Emotional and Behavioural Effects

The application of aversive training techniques has been shown to increase stress, anxiety, and fear in dogs (Ziv, 2017). These emotional states can have detrimental effects on a dog's quality of life, leading to maladaptive behaviours such as hyper-vigilance, avoidance, or aggression. Chronic stress associated with punishment-based methods can also impair learning, making it more challenging to teach alternative behaviours (Hiby, Rooney, & Bradshaw, 2004).


The Case for Positive Reinforcement in Snake Avoidance Training

Positive reinforcement offers a more humane, effective, and scientifically supported alternative to punishment-based training methods. This approach focuses on reinforcing desirable behaviours, thereby empowering dogs to make appropriate choices in their environment.


Teaching Alternative Behaviours

Rather than focusing on what a dog should not do (e.g., engage with a snake), positive reinforcement emphasises teaching what the dog should do instead. For example:

  • Coming Away from the Snake: The dog is trained to move away from the snake and return to their handler upon encountering it.

  • Focusing on the Handler: The dog learns to remain engaged with their handler, ignoring environmental distractions, including snakes.

By reinforcing these desirable behaviours, the dog develops a reliable response to the presence of a snake without experiencing fear or distress.


Extinction and Behaviour Change

Extinction involves removing the reinforcement that previously maintained an undesirable behaviour. For example, if a dog is prevented from practicing snake-chasing behaviour and simultaneously taught an alternative behaviour (e.g., recall), the undesirable behaviour will naturally decrease over time. Unlike punishment, extinction does not rely on aversive stimuli, making it a more ethical and sustainable approach to behaviour modification.


Practical Application of Positive Reinforcement in Snake Avoidance


Step 1: Building a Reliable Recall

  • Begin in a controlled, distraction-free environment.

  • Call the dog’s name, reinforce them with high-value treats for responding, and gradually increase the level of distraction.

  • Over time, the dog learns that returning to the handler results in positive outcomes, reinforcing the recall behaviour.


Step 2: Controlled Exposure to Snake Stimuli

  • Introduce a realistic snake model or snake scent in a safe and controlled setting.

  • Allow the dog to notice the stimulus. When they disengage or focus on their handler, immediately reinforce the desired behaviour.

  • This process teaches the dog that moving away from the snake leads to positive reinforcement.


Step 3: Gradual Increase in Challenge

  • As the dog becomes more proficient, increase the difficulty by adding movement to the snake model or introducing the stimulus in a natural environment.

  • Maintain consistent reinforcement for the desired behaviour, ensuring that the dog’s focus remains on the handler rather than the snake.


Step 4: Proofing the Behaviour

  • Practice in a variety of environments to generalise the behaviour.

  • Ensure the dog can respond reliably under different conditions, including varying levels of distraction.


Benefits of Positive Reinforcement

  1. Improved Emotional Well-Being: Positive reinforcement fosters confidence and reduces fear, promoting emotional resilience in dogs.

  2. Stronger Handler-Dog Bond: Dogs trained with positive reinforcement are more likely to trust and engage with their handlers.

  3. Lasting Behaviour Change: By addressing the root cause of behaviour and reinforcing alternatives, positive reinforcement creates long-term results.

  4. Ethical Training Practices: Positive reinforcement respects the dog as a sentient being, prioritising their mental and physical well-being.


Conclusion

Snake avoidance training is essential for ensuring a dog’s safety, particularly in regions where encounters with snakes are common. However, the choice of training methodology has profound implications for the dog’s behaviour, emotional state, and quality of life.

Punishment-based methods, while widely used, fail to address the underlying causes of undesirable behaviours and often result in unintended negative consequences, including fear, anxiety, and mistrust. Positive reinforcement, by contrast, provides an evidence-based, humane alternative that empowers dogs to make appropriate choices while preserving their emotional well-being.


The question is not whether dogs can be trained to avoid snakes—it is how we choose to train them. As science and ethics evolve, so should our training practices, ensuring that they reflect the best interests of our canine companions.


References

  1. Skinner, B.F. (1938). The Behaviour of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis.

  2. Ziv, G. (2017). The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour, 19, 50-60.

  3. Azrin, N.H., & Holz, W.C. (1966). Punishment. In W.K. Honig (Ed.), Operant Behaviour: Areas of Research and Application.

  4. Hiby, E.F., Rooney, N.J., & Bradshaw, J.W.S. (2004). Dog training methods: Their use, effectiveness, and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Animal Welfare, 13(1), 63-69.

Recent Posts

See All

Commentaires


CASI
Badge to show qualification
COAPE Centre of Applied Pet Ethology

Why choose us

As an industry expert with extensive experience in pet care and behaviour, I am here to provide you with personalised guidance to better understand your pet’s unique needs.

Whether you’re dealing with common challenges or more complex behavioural issues, I can offer practical solutions and actionable advice tailored to your situation.

My goal is to help you build a stronger bond with your pet while ensuring their well-being and happiness.

Perth's Animal Training Specialists

8 Woodland Loop

Edgewater, 6027. W.A

 

Telephone - 0438-998-204

ABN - 2052 842 8773

Contact us

Thanks! Message sent.

Let's get social!  🐶🐱🐎💖

  • Facebook Social Icon
  • Instagram Social Icon
  • Twitter Social Icon
  • YouTube Social  Icon
  • Pinterest Social Icon
bottom of page